How to make kids learn faster:
Hacking our
senses to boost learning power
(Thinkstock)
Some schools are pumping music,
noises and fragrances into the classroom to see if it improves exam results –
could it work?
What
did your school smell like? Was it noisy or peaceful?
It
might not seem important, but a growing body of research suggests that smells
and sounds can have an impact on learning, performance and creativity. Indeed,
some head teachers have recently taken to broadcasting noises and pumping
whiffs into their schools to see whether it can boost grades. Is there anything
in it? And if so, what are the implications for the way we all work and study?
There
is certainly some well-established research to suggest that some noises can
have a detrimental effect on learning. Numerous studies over the past 15 years
have found that children attending schools under the flight paths of large
airports lag behind in their exam results.
But
general noise seems to have an effect too. Bridget Shield, a professor of
acoustics at London South Bank University, and Julie Dockrell, now at the Institute
of Education, have been conducting studies and advising politicians on the
effects of all sorts of noises, such as traffic and sirens, as well as noise
generated by the children themselves. When they recreated those particular
sounds in an experimental setting whilst children completed various cognitive
tasks, they found a significant negative effect on exam scores. “Everything
points to a detrimental impact of the noise on children’s performance, in
numeracy, in literacy, and in spelling,” says Shield. The noise seemed to have
an especially detrimental effect on children with special needs. `
Shield
says the sound of “babble” – the chatter of other children, is particularly
distracting in the classroom. Architects that fashion open-plan classrooms in
schools would do well to take this on board. “People are very distracted by
speech – particularly if it’s understandable, but you’re not involved in it.”
This phenomenon is also known as the irrelevant speech effect, she says, adding
that “it’s a very common finding in open-plan offices as well.”
Whether
background sounds are beneficial or not seems to depend on what kind of noise
it is – and the volume. In a series of studies published last year, Ravi Mehta
from the College of Business at Illinois and colleagues tested people’s
creativity while exposed to a soundtrack made up of background noises – such as
coffee-shop chatter and construction-site drilling – at different volumes. They
found that people were more creative when the background noises were played at
a medium level than when volume was low. Loud background noise, however,
damaged their creativity.
This
makes sense for a couple of reasons, says psychologist Dr Nick Perham, at
Cardiff Metropolitan University in the UK, who studies the effect of sounds on
learning but was not involved in the study. Firstly, he says, sounds that
are most distracting tend to be very variable. A general hum in the
background suggests a steady-state sound with not much acoustical variation.
“So there’s not much there to capture your attention – nothing distracting the
subjects,” he says. At the same time, the background noise might cause the
subjects to be in a slightly heightened state of arousal, says Perham. You
don’t want too much or too little arousal. “Medium arousal is best for good
performance. So it might be that a general hum in the background gives an
optimum level of arousal.” With that in mind, Perham suggests there may be some
benefit to playing music or other sounds in an art class or other situations where
creativity is key.
Many
teachers all over the world already play music to students in class. Many are
inspired by the belief that hearing music can boost IQ in subsequent tasks, the so-called Mozart effect. While the evidence
actually suggests it’s a stretch to say classical music boosts brainpower,
researchers do think pleasant sounds before a task can sometimes lift your mood
and help you perform well, says Perham, who has done his own studies on the
phenomenon. The key appears to be that you enjoy what you’re hearing. “If you
like the music or you like the sound – even listening to a Stephen King novel –
then you did better. It didn’t matter about the music,” he says.
However,
it’s worth considering that music is not always helpful while you’re trying to
work. Trying to perform a task which involves serial recall – for instance,
doing mental arithmetic – will be impaired by sounds with acoustic variation,
which includes most types of music, says Perham. (Except a few, like extreme
death metal.) Songs with lyrics, on the other hand, are more likely to
interfere with tasks that involve semantics – such as reading comprehension.
“The task and the sound are important, when you have both of them using the
same process then you get problems,” he says.
So, it
seems that schools that choose to screen out disturbing noises and create
positive soundscapes could enhance the learning of their students, so long as
they make careful choices.
This
isn’t the only sense being tweaked to affect learning. Special educational
needs students at Sydenham high school in London are being encouraged to revise
different subjects in the presence of different smells – grapefruit scents for
maths, lavender for French and spearmint for history.
Less
research has gone into the idea of whether scents can help with cognitive
performance, although there have been intriguing findings. In 2003,
psychologist Mark Moss, at Northumbria University, carried out a range of
cognitive tests on subjects who were exposed
either to lavender or rosemary aromas. “Rosemary in particular
caught my attention as it is considered to be arousing and linked to memory,”
he says, whereas lavender is considered to be sedating. Moss found that those
who were smelling lavender performed significantly worse in working memory
tests, and had impaired reaction times for both memory and attention-based
tasks, compared to controls. Those in the rosemary group, on the other hand,
did much better than controls overall in the memory tasks, although their
reaction times were slower.
Why
might this be? It’s perhaps not surprising that smells affect memory, given
that the brain’s olfactory bulb is intimately linked to the hippocampus, which
deals with learning. But Moss suspected there was more to it. To explore the
pharmacological effects of rosemary on the body, he drew blood samples from
volunteers who had just undergone cognitive tests in a rosemary-infused room,
and found that they had elevated levels of a compound called 1,8-cineole in
their blood. Previous research has shown that this compound increases
communication between brain cells, which might explain how it improves brain function.
So, as you finish reading this story, take a moment to tune into your senses. Close your eyes and take a few nice deep breaths. What can you hear and smell? The answer, it seems, may affect how much you learnt in the past few minutes.
So, as you finish reading this story, take a moment to tune into your senses. Close your eyes and take a few nice deep breaths. What can you hear and smell? The answer, it seems, may affect how much you learnt in the past few minutes.
Source: BBC
Note: If you are an entrepreneur (create your own business) , creative (projects )or need money for an important cause (hospital bills, school fees, birthdays parties, funeral, marriage etc) you must understand how to raise funds via social network see Crowdfunding sites
No comments:
Post a Comment